Skip to main content
  • Commentary
  • Published:

Responses to FTO genetic test feedback for obesity in a sample of overweight adults: a qualitative analysis

Abstract

Current evidence indicates that genetic testing for obesity risk has limited affective or behavioral impact, but few studies have explored the effects among individuals who self-identify as having weight problems. Here, we report findings from in-depth telephone interviews with seven overweight or obese volunteers who were genotyped for one weight-related gene (FTO), which may offer interesting insights into motivations to seek out genetic testing and immediate reactions to it. All participants had a BMI > 25. The gene test identified one participant as homozygous for the ‘higher-risk’ variant (AA), three heterozygous (AT), and three homozygous for the ‘lower-risk’ variant (TT) of FTO. All participants said they took part to find an explanation for their personal struggle with weight control. Those with one or two higher-risk variants experienced relief and saw the result as confirming their private assumption that they were susceptible to weight gain for reasons perceived as ‘external’ to them. However, at the same time, they described themselves as more motivated to overcome their genetic predisposition. Those with lower-risk variants reported brief disappointment, but then focused on alternative explanations, reinforcing the multifactorial nature of obesity. Despite objectively low ‘information value,’ all individuals derived some ‘personal’ benefit from FTO genetic test feedback. However, improving education about the multifactorial nature of complex conditions is important to decrease polarized thinking and associated genetic determinism and stigma to derive the greatest benefits of novel genetic technologies for individuals and their health.

Obesity is a highly heritable condition with a complex etiology, probably comprising interactions between many genes of small effect and the ‘obesogenic’ environment (O’Rahilly and Farooqi 2008; Speakman and O’Rahilly 2012). FTO was the first ‘common obesity’ gene to be identified (Frayling et al. 2007; Scuteri et al. 2007). In population samples, the higher-risk variant (A) is associated with modestly higher body weight (1.2 kg per allele), and AA homozygotes have a 20 % higher lifetime risk of overweight or obesity compared with TT (lower-risk) homozygotes, although all genotypes are observed at all body weights. The function of FTO is beginning to be characterized, possibly influencing traits such as appetite avidity and satiety responsiveness (Llewellyn et al. 2012; O’Rahilly and Farooqi 2008; Wardle et al. 2008; Karra et al. 2013).

Despite uncertain clinical utility due to small effect sizes, the obesity genes identified to date are included in consumer-based genetic testing panels (e.g., www.23andme.com; www.pathway.com). However, debate continues in the wider genetics community about the benefits and harms of feedback for conditions with complex, multifactorial etiology (Grosse et al. 2009; Evans et al. 2011; Frueh et al. 2011). Supporters of testing anticipate that a higher-risk result might serve as a ‘wake-up call,’ motivating beneficial lifestyle changes (Collins 2006). Those cautioning against testing argue that genetic determinism means that a higher risk result could lead to fatalism and discourage efforts at behavior change (Marteau and Weinman 2006). This latter response would be particularly problematic for obesity where prevention and treatment depend primarily on behavior change.

The potential for harm from fatalistic responses makes it important to investigate the psychological impact of genetic test feedback for common, complex conditions. Vignette studies, where participants ‘imagine’ receiving genetic test feedback, were a first step. They typically find that people imagine that a higher risk results would make them more motivated to change their behavior, and there is little evidence of negative emotional or motivational outcomes (Sanderson and Wardle 2005; Frosch et al. 2005; Meisel et al. 2012; Meisel and Wardle 2013). However, although these results are encouraging, they cannot be assumed to generalize to real feedback conditions.

One small study incorporated feedback for the b3AR gene into a weight-loss program for obese women (Harvey-Berino et al. 2001). Similar to the results of hypothetical studies, women receiving a higher-risk result remained motivated to change their behavior without any indication of fatalism, although there was no comparison group receiving a negative genetic test result. A randomized controlled trial giving feedback for familial hypercholesterolemia (FH) also found no evidence that higher-risk genetic test feedback resulted in fatalism or reductions in dietary adherence (Marteau et al. 2004). However, consistent with a recent Cochrane review of genetic test feedback for a range of health behaviors (Marteau et al. 2010), neither study found any impact on behavior.

Studies investigating impact of lower-risk genetic test results are scarce. In the FH trial, those who had no genetic predisposition to FH perceived the disease as less controllable, and thought they would have less control over heart disease, although the differences were no longer evident at the 6-month follow-up (Marteau et al. 2004). To our knowledge, this is the only study explicitly commenting on the effect of lower-risk genetic test results.

The first large study to investigate effects of genetic testing for multiple conditions included over 2,000 individuals who received a reduced rate purchase of the ‘Navigenics Health Compass,’ which gives results for 23 conditions, in exchange for responding to surveys 6 and 12 months after receiving the results. Regardless of risk status, there was no change in anxiety nor any evidence of positive or negative behavior change either at short- or long-term follow-up. However, a ‘healthy participant bias’ among these early adopters of genetic testing may have contributed to the null finding (Bloss et al. 2011, 2013).

Although research has begun to explore the emotional and behavioral outcomes of genetic test feedback, few studies have addressed the question of why it has less impact (positive or negative) than anticipated. Here, we describe findings from in-depth telephone interviews with seven overweight or obese white British women (BMI range 25–39 kg/m2), aged 34–54 years (mean age = 45, median age = 44), who volunteered to be genotyped for FTO.

Ethical approval for the study was granted by the UCL Research Ethics Committee for non-NHS research (ID Number 2471/001). Communication about the study was conducted largely by email (including result disclosure) to resemble the format used by companies offering consumer-based genetic test feedback. Before enrollment, and when receiving their FTO genetic test result, participants received written information materials, which clearly outlined the multifactorial nature of obesity and the modest contribution of FTO. Furthermore, population frequencies of FTO were described to ensure that participants understood that obesity can occur despite having the lower-risk genotype. This information was verbally reiterated during the interviews. One participant was homozygous for the higher-risk A variant (AA), three were heterozygous (AT), and three were homozygous for the lower-risk variant (TT). All participants correctly recalled their test result and could explain its meaning in their own words.

In the interviews, it soon transpired that despite being aware about obesity as a multifactorial disease, people’s motivation to participate in this study was unanimously driven by the desire to find an ‘explanation’ for their weight status. For example, one participant took part to find out whether there are genetic reasons why I find it extremely difficult to lose weight or why I am larger than other people, is there a reason why, other than the fact that I do love food? (P7, TT). Consistent with this, receiving a lower-risk genetic test result led to brief disappointment, perhaps because despite knowing ‘rationally’ about the many causes of obesity and FTO’s modest contribution, people anticipated and hoped for the higher-risk FTO genotype. However, the disappointment did not last because all TT participants spontaneously drew on their knowledge about the alternative causes of obesity; for example: It’s not all down to genetics because it’s down to the environment, the way you have been brought up and all that that has an influence, I think, on you, the choices that you make and stuff like that, you know’ (P6, TT). Another pointed out that the contribution of FTO is only small and that there may be other genes that are the cause of their weight gain: ‘And it doesn’t make a lot of difference having it. Because if you have got it you are 3 kilos, 7.3 pound, yeah, no, it’s a very small amount […]. I still think that there’s something in the genes, but not in these particular ones, which are simply related to body build […] (P7, TT). One participant also considered potential advantages from not having the ‘higher risk’ variant, demonstrating evidence for positive active coping: ‘[…] in a way I should be pleased because then, you know, there’s less things that stop you from losing weight because if I did have the gene, maybe it’s harder to lose weight.’ (P6, TT).

Although those receiving AA and AT results also held multifaceted beliefs about the causes of obesity, these were only shared once prompted. They felt ‘pleased with the result,’ although it came as ‘no surprise, because it just confirmed what I felt anyway’ (P4, AA). One participant described her AT result as ‘normal’ genotype and explained that she was ‘living that result’ (P1, AT), perhaps reflecting the understanding that overweight is caused by a combination of genes and environment.

Regardless of their test result, many participants were quick to note that they never intended to use it an as ‘excuse’ for their weight status, perhaps reflecting acceptance of broader societal attitudes toward obesity as a personal shortcoming for which the individual is to blame. Instead, having a genetic ‘explanation’ helped confirm the perception that forces beyond personal responsibility contributed to their difficulties with healthy body weight maintenance: ‘I understand that genetics and the things that we are likely to choose as a result of our genes or how we are likely to feel means that we can’t always help the fact that we eat what we eat, if that makes sense’ (P4, AA). Knowledge of the underlying genetics appeared to alleviate some of the guilt and stigma associated with overweight. In this context, some participants also mentioned that there might be value in popularizing the message that genetics is a contributor to overweight, because ‘it could make some people more understanding, it would perhaps make the medical profession more understanding’ (P7, TT).

Although the findings presented here are from a small volunteer sample, they nonetheless offer interesting insights into people’s motivation to seek out genetic test feedback and their immediate reactions to it. Currently, consumer-based genetic tests emphasize the predictive value of genetic testing in their marketing strategies. However, these results suggest that the desire for an ‘etiological explanation’ of a condition may be an alternative driver for seeking out genetic testing, a hypothesis that could be explored in further research.

In line with findings from previous hypothetical and clinical studies (Meisel et al. 2012; Conradt et al. 2009), the results suggest that genetic test feedback for obesity risk may have beneficial psychological effects for overweight and obese individuals beyond ‘objective’ clinical utility. Despite the objectively low ‘information value’ of genetic risk information for an existing condition, people appear to derive ‘personal’ psychological benefits from the information.

There is often concern that a focus on genetics in disease etiology fosters deterministic views, and this did not appear to be the case. Far from resulting in fatalism, an ‘explanation’ for weight status in the form of a higher-risk result appeared to motivate a shift of focus toward taking action (beating my biology), while a lower-risk result helped reinforce understanding about the multiple causes of obesity. However, we were careful to emphasize the complex origin of obesity, and FTO’s modest effects, throughout the study, which may have protected participants from engaging in genetic determinism. Furthermore, the participants were volunteers and may have selected themselves into the study based on their anticipated positive reaction to the genetic test result, an issue that is common to studies in this area (Sanderson and Wardle 2008).

In clinical practice, genetic feedback for a single gene conferring very moderate risk is unlikely to be used, not least because feedback for gene panels is now feasible and cost-effective and whole-genome sequencing is eagerly anticipated. Reactions to genetic test results from panel or whole-genome sequencing may differ because participants may assign a different meaning to these results. However, as findings presented here match those obtained from earlier studies, and from other areas of genetic testing and health, behavior change irrespective of whether genetic feedback was given for one or multiple conditions (e.g., Leventhal et al. 1997; Harvey-Berino et al. 2001; McBride et al. 2002; Marteau et al. 2004; Sanderson and Wardle 2005; Bloss et al. 2011; Hollands et al. 2012; Grant et al. 2013). On this basis, they provide some reassurance that people are generally unlikely to misinterpret and overstate the impact of genetic test results, although the limitations of self-selection have to be kept in mind.

With increasing understanding about the mechanisms through which genes affect behavior, it will be interesting to investigate whether behavioral advice tailored to the individual’s genotype will be superior to giving individuals generic advice for behavior change. For example, FTO is thought to affect weight gain partly through low satiety sensitivity and high food responsiveness (Wardle et al. 2008; Karra et al. 2013), but no study to date has explored the benefits of information focusing on these specific characteristics. Furthermore, it will be important to carry on improving education about the genetic and non-genetic causes of multifactorial conditions not only to avoid genetic determinism, but also to diminish stigma in order to derive the greatest benefits of novel genetic technologies for individuals and their health.

References

  • Bloss CS, Schork NJ, Topol EJ (2011) Effect of direct-to-consumer genomewide profiling to assess disease risk. N Engl J Med 364:524–534

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloss CS, Wineinger NE, Darst BF, Schork NJ, Topol EJ (2013) Impact of direct-to-consumer genomic testing at long term follow-up. J Med Gen. Retrieved http://jmg.bmj.com/content/early/2013/04/03/jmedgenet-2012-101207.abstract

  • Collins FS (2006) The human genome project and the future of medicine. Ann NY Acad Sci 882:42–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conradt M, Dierk JM, Schlumberger P, Albohn C, Rauh E, Hinney A et al (2009) A consultation with genetic information about obesity decreases self-blame about eating and leads to realistic weight loss goals in obese individuals. J Psychosom Res 66:287–295

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans JP, Meslin EM, Marteau TM, Caulfield T (2011) Deflating the genomic bubble. Science 331:861–862

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frayling TM, Timpson NJ, Weedon MN, Zeggini E, Freathy RM, Lindgren CM et al (2007) A common variant in the FTO gene is associated with body mass index and predisposes to childhood and adult obesity. Science 316:889–894

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frosch DL, Mello P, Lerman C (2005) Behavioral consequences of testing for obesity risk. Cancer Epidemol Biomarkers Prev 14:1485–1489

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frueh FW, Greely HT, Green RC, Hogarth S, Siegel S (2011) The future of direct-to-consumer clinical genetic tests. Nat Rev Gen 12:511–515

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grant RW, O’Brien KE, Waxler JL, Vassy JL, Delahanty LM, Bissett LG et al (2013) Personalized genetic risk counseling to motivate diabetes prevention a randomized trial. Diabetes care 36:13–19

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grosse SD, McBride CM, Evans JP, Khoury MJ (2009) Personal utility and genomic information: look before you leap. Genet Med 11:575–576

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey-Berino J, Gold EC, West DS, Shuldiner AR, Walston J, Starling RD et al (2001) Does genetic testing for obesity influence confidence in the ability to lose weight? A pilot investigation. J Am Diet Assoc 101:1351–1353

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollands GJ, Sophia CLW, Richard AP, Natalie JP, Alastair F, Jeremy S et al (2012) Effect of communicating DNA based risk assessments for Crohn’s disease on smoking cessation: randomised controlled trial. BMJ 345:e4708. doi:10.1136/bmj.e4708

  • Karra E, O’Daly OG, Choudhury AI, Yousseif A, Millership S, Neary MT et al (2013) A link between FTO, ghrelin, and impaired brain food-cue responsivity. J Clin Invest 123(8):3539

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leventhal H, Benyamini L, Brownlee S, Diefenbach M, Leventhal E, Patrick-Miller L (eds.) (1997) Perceptions of health and illness: current research and applications. In: Illness representations: theoretical foundations. Harwood Publishers Ltd, London, p 19–45

  • Llewellyn CH, van Jaarsveld CH, Plomin R, Fisher A, Wardle J (2012) Inherited behavioral susceptibility to adiposity in infancy: a multivariate genetic analysis of appetite and weight in the Gemini birth cohort. Am J Clin Nutr 95:633–639

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McBride CM, Bepler G, Lipkus IM, Lyna P, Samsa G, Albright J, Rimer BK (2002) Incorporating genetic susceptibility feedback into a smoking cessation program for African-American smokers with low income. Cancer Epidem Biomar 11(6):521–528

    Google Scholar 

  • Marteau TM, Weinman J (2006) Self-regulation and the behavioral response to DNA risk information: a theoretical analysis and framework for future research. Soc Sci Med 62:1360–1368

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marteau T, Senior V, Humphries SE, Bobrow M, Cranston T, Crook MA et al (2004) Psychological impact of genetic testing for familial hypercholesterolemia within a previously aware population: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Med Genet A 128A:285–293

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marteau TM, French DP, Griffin SJ, Prevost AT, Sutton S, Watkinson C et al (2010) Effects of communicating DNA-based disease risk estimates on risk-reducing behaviors. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 10

  • Meisel SF, Wardle J (2013) ‘Battling my Biology’: psychological effects of genetic testing for risk of weight gain. J Genet Counsel 1–8. doi:10.1007/s10897-013-9628-9

  • Meisel SF, Walker C, Wardle J (2012) Psychological responses to genetic testing for weight gain: a vignette study. Obesity 20:540–546

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Rahilly S, Farooqi IS (2008) Human obesity: a heritable neurobehavioral disorder that is highly sensitive to environmental conditions. Diabetes 57:2905–2910

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanderson SC, Wardle J (2005) Will genetic testing for complex diseases increase motivation to quit smoking? Anticipated reactions in a survey of smokers. Health Educ Behav 32:640–653

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanderson SC, Wardle J (2008) Associations between anticipated reactions to genetic test results and interest in genetic testing: will self-selection reduce the potential for harm? Genet Test 12:59–66

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scuteri A, Sanna S, Chen WM, Uda M, Albai G, Strait J et al (2007) Genome-wide association scan shows genetic variants in the FTO gene are associated with obesity-related traits. PLoS Genet 3:115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Speakman JR, O’Rahilly S (2012) Fat: an evolving issue. Dis Model Mech 5:569–573

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wardle J, Carnell S, Haworth CM, Farooqi IS, O’Rahilly S, Plomin R (2008) Obesity associated genetic variation in FTO is associated with diminished satiety. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 93:3640–3643

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant from Cancer Research UK (C1418/A10843). We would like to thank Professor Sir Stephen O’Rahilly and Professor Sadaf Farooqi from Cambridge University, Institute of Metabolic Sciences, Cambridge, UK for their helpful and constructive advice, and David Withers for the DNA analysis.

Conflict of interest

None.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to S. F. Meisel.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Meisel, S.F., Wardle, J. Responses to FTO genetic test feedback for obesity in a sample of overweight adults: a qualitative analysis. Genes Nutr 9, 374 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12263-013-0374-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12263-013-0374-2

Keywords